/Vaccination

Primary LanguageJavaScriptMIT LicenseMIT

Vaccination

Team Members:

Kerem Cerit, Azim Selvi, Lea Cohausz

Research Question

Does the percentage of the population already vaccinated and the infection rate of a virus influence people regarding their decision towards vaccination?

Introduction and Theory

The Covid-19-pandemic and the vaccine to overcome it are on everyone's mind. Though the news of successful vaccine trials and its subsequent admission in the United States and then in the European Union were celebrated, a quite substantial percentage of the population seems to feel wary regarding the decision to get vaccinated themselves with the Süddeutsche Zeitung even dubbing it "Die Vertrauensfrage", i.e., "The question of trust" (Becker 2020, Charisius 2020). The question is, how willing people are to get vaccinated with a vaccine with limited long-term experience. Even people who are generally advocating vaccination seem uncertain regarding this specific situation (Becker 2020).

Previous vaccination research often assumes free-riding - when many people have been vaccinated already, a person does not have to get vaccinated him-/herself and will, nonetheless, be protected (Ibuka et al. 2014). However, the theory of free-riding is usually employed in cases where a large percentage of the population is vaccinated and has been for a while. Nonetheless, some people currently argue in a similar manner when saying that they will wait for others to be vaccinated first (Charisius 2020). Their reason for this decision is not explicitly to wait until it is no longer necessary for them to receive the vaccine - though implicitly they do assume that the current crisis gets resolved if others get vaccinated - but rather to be certain that there are no negative consequences of receiving it. This, we argue, is another form of free-riding at play (where the risks are unequally distributed), though, not the one traditionally researched.

Other scholars showed and argued for more complex processes relating to the decision in favor of receiving a vaccine (Streefland et al.1999, Streefland 2001). For example, people are influenced by other people in their social circle and are more likely to get vaccinated when they do (bandwagon effect). They are also more likely to get vaccinated if they are educated both on the disease's risks as well as on the vaccine. Again, the current situation only allows the latter in a limited manner and thus makes it very different from anything previously studied. The former, however, is probably very much given currently. Connected to this, an important aspect is the perceived risk for oneself and others and with the a new and potentially even more infectious Covid-19-variant discovered (Zeit 2020), people should be more willing to receive the vaccine, if this theory holds true.

Thus, given the previous research results and the current ever-changing, new situation, we ask the following question: Does the percentage of the population already vaccinated and the infection rate of a virus influence people regarding their decision towards vaccination?

As infection rate we define the number of people an infected person infects on average. The easier a disease spreads, the higher the risk of being affected by it oneself (and by spreading it to others).

Hypotheses

Given the two kinds of free-riding introduced earlier, we argue that people are at first waiting for other people's experiences with the vaccine before getting vaccinated themselves and that when many have been vaccinated already, they again do not vaccinate because of the traditional free-riding phenomenon. Thus, our first hypothesis is:

H1: If the percentage of the vaccinated population increases up to a certain point, more individuals decide in favor of vaccination. From then on, the relationship turns negative (i.e. the more people are vaccinated, the fewer individuals decide in favor of vaccination).

Furthermore, in accordance with Streefland (2001) we assume that an increased risk awareness drives people towards vaccination, thus:

H2: If the infection rate is high, more people will get vaccinated.

To test the above hypotheses, we will conduct a vignette online experiment varying the two independent variables.

Experiment

Variables and Treatment

The dependent variable will be a dummy variable that shows whether a person says that he/she would get vaccinated or not. The independent variables will be the infection rate and the percentage of the population which is already vaccinated. There are three different conditions of the infection rate (1, 5, 10), and five different conditions for the percentage of the population already vaccinated (5%, 23%, 51%, 72%, 95%). The respondents will always receive a combination of the two treatment variables.

Furthermore, we will control for certain aspects. Among our control variables are measures for how involved people are in the lives of family members and whether they spend time volunteering. We assume that both of these variables will increase people's willingness to get vaccinated either to protect others (family) or because of a sense of community duty (volunteering). We also tentatively ask about their medical history. Regularly going to the doctor or having a chronic disease should increase people's willingness to vaccinate as should a generally health-aware lifestyle. As medical information is a very sensitive topic and could lead to people dropping out of the survey, we only ask participants how they would rate their own health within a 1 to 10 scale and how regularly they exercise. Additionally, we ask for respondent's trust in the government and large companies. We believe that people distrustful to those entities generally are less likely to trust a new vaccine admitted by those entities.

As is norm, we ask for a number of demographic information (such as age, gender, etc.). Two of the variables we are especially interested in are the respondent's level of education and income. In particular the first one is often assumed to be important with a higher level of education is usually associated with a higher willingness to get vaccinated. Income is asked for by giving people categories to choose from. There, they can say where they would place themselves regarding their income in relation to that of others in their country (e.g., "middle upper-class income level"). This is done in that manner, because this is also a traditionally sensitive topic and because we aim to ask people in many different countries making a comparison otherwise difficult.

Survey Construction

As already mentioned, we decided to test out our hypotheses using a vignette experiment. The questionnaire is split up in several parts. After an introduction to the survey where we mention the topic and expected duration, we ask respondents for some general socio-demographic information. Then, we explain important terms - such as infection rate - to them and test their understanding to make sure that the respondents will give informed answers. Afterwards, we explain the specific scenario to them and, again, check their understanding. The participants will then be given an infection rate and a percentage of the population vaccinated and will be asked, whether they would get vaccinated. They will be given new combinations four more times and will be asked for their decision every time. We decided to give them multiple treatments - instead of just one as it is usually done - so that we do not have as high requirements relating to the number of people necessary to participate to get meaningful results and because we think that every potential effect of receiving one kind of treatment first and then the next will average out. We decided against using more than five treatments per participant so that people do not get bored and exit the survey. After they have completed this part, they will be asked why they decided the way they did, before some more control variables will be covered.

Vignettes

Our vignettes look like this:

"In your country, a virus breaks out. The virus is the main cause of disease B which can lead to several symptoms such as fever, coughing, nausea, severe headache. In later stages, Disease B proves to be deadly with a death rate of 2%.

According to the experts, the virus that causes Disease B has an infection rate of 1. This means that every infected person on average infects 1 other person.

There exists a vaccine that has already proven to be effective against the vaccine. This vaccine has already been given to 5% of the population."

The first paragraph of the vignette stays the same for all treatments. Only the name of the disease varies depending on the infection rate. In the middle and last paragraph, the infection rate and the vaccinated percentage of the population is altered.

Understanding Checks

We try to make sure that we only receive serious and informed responses by including two understanding checks and a timer. The timer is set in such a way that if a person takes too long to answer a question (more than two hours) his/her answers are discarded. The understanding checks appear after we explain some basic terms and after we introduce the scenario. A participant cannot continue unless he or she answered the understanding checks correctly. Understanding checks may have the disadvantage that people feel annoyed by them, but as the quality of the answers is of great importance to us, we take this risk.

We aim to prevent people from dropping out by asking potentially sensitive questions (such as the questions regarding income and health) in a hopefully also sensitive way. We hope that this is sufficient to allow people to share their information. However, by asking the questions in the way we do, the information we receive back is less detailed and clear. We believe that the risk of too many people dropping out or them feeling uncomfortable is much greater than the risk of receiving lower quality information.

Analysis

Once we have a sufficient amount of data, we can start with the analysis. As we have a dichotomous dependent variable (vaccinate - 1, not vaccinate - 0), we choose logistic regression as our means of analysis. Every model must include our two treatments - infection rate and percentage of the population vaccinated - as independent variables. As these are categorical variables with ordering, they will be recoded to values 1 to 3 and 1 to 5 (1 representing the smallest and 5 the largest number) respectively. Furthermore, H1 requires a quadratic term of the percentage of vaccinated population to be able to observe the effect we expect. The control variables will also be preprocessed and added. For hypothesis 1 to be true, the variable percentage of the population vaccinated needs to be positive and significant and its squared term needs to be negative and significant. For H2 to be true, the variable infection rate needs to be positive and significant.

Limits and Unresolved Issues

However, our vignette study also is limited in its informative value and there are some unresolved issues. First, in contrast to a classical experimental design, we simply ask respondents if they would get vaccinated. This means that we can't be really sure, if the respondents would act in accordance with their answer in the real-world. One reason for this might be, that the respondents could be influenced by social desirability effects and therefore alter their answers. This could go in either direction, meaning that some individuals may want to appear altruistic and tell us that they will get vaccinated even though they would not; Or that alternatively, some people could tell us that they would not get vaccinated, even though they would in real-life. The latter could be possible in situations were the percentage of the population vaccinated is very high and the infection rate very low. Respondents may not want to appear fearful and report to us that they would not get the vaccine. Either way, this would lead to biased results. Additionally, the vignette design does not consider all the various information and experiences the respondents have in a real-life setting, especially if we think about the current Covid-19 pandemic and the media's coverage (Gozzi et al., 2020).

Second, we can't confidently deduce the motivation behind the answers which the respondents give us about their vaccination decisions. Even though we include an option to explain why the respondents decided the way they did, it is highly unrealistic that we get a satisfying answer from most of the respondents in a way that we understand if the person acted – in his/her mind – selfishly or altruistic. And for those answers which are well explained, we also have the danger of social desirability. This means, that the interpretation of potential results is limited insofar as one can't interpret the underlying mechanisms of these results.

Third, we do not know where the exact "cutoff" point for every respondent is. We define the cutoff point as a certain value of the variables population vaccinated and infection rate at which a respondent changes his/her behavior. For example, in our first hypothesis, we state that at a certain percentage point of the vaccinated population, respondents would theoretically change their vaccination behavior. This would be a cutoff point. Since population vaccinated and infection rate can theoretically take on infinite numbers, we just hardly can predict where certain cutoff points are. We only ask for Y infection rate and Z percentages of the population vaccinated. Moreover, the interaction between our two independent variables makes it even harder to find the true cutoff point. This issue is even more exacerbated, when we think about, that every respondents has an individual cutoff point.
While we are aware of these problems, we nonetheless believe that our study is a good starting point to address limits of previous research that we highlighted in the introduction, e.g., we consider the - realistic yet barely researched - scenario of having a very low rate of the population previously vaccinated.

References

Becker, K.B. (2020). "Die andere Hälfte überzeugen". Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung. [Last Accessed: 22.12.2020.] https://www.faz.net/aktuell/politik/inland/aerztin-heidrun-thais-will-zur-corona-impfung-motivieren-17109316.html.

Charisius, H. (2020). Die Vertrauensfrage. Süddeutsche Zeitung. [Last Accessed: 22.12.2020.] https://www.sueddeutsche.de/wissen/coronavirus-impfung-deutschland-1.5151552?reduced=true.

Gozzi, N., Tizzani, M., Starnini, M., Ciulla, F., Paolotti, D., Panisson, A., & Perra, N. (2020). Collective Response to Media Coverage of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Reddit and Wikipedia: Mixed-Methods Analysis. Journal of medical Internet research, 22(10), e21597.

Ibuka, Y., Li, M., Vietri, J., Chapman, G. B., & Galvani, A. P. (2014). Free-riding behavior in vaccination decisions: an experimental study. PloS one, 9(1), e87164.

Streefland, P. H. (2001). Public doubts about vaccination safety and resistance against vaccination. Health policy, 55(3), 159-172.

Streefland, P., Chowdhury, A. M. R., & Ramos-Jimenez, P. (1999). Patterns of vaccination acceptance. Social science & medicine, 49(12), 1705-1716.

Zeit Online. (2020). Britische Corona-Mutation wohl deutlich ansteckender. Zeit. [Last Accessed: 22.12.2020.] https://www.zeit.de/wissen/gesundheit/2020-12/christian-drosten-coronavirus-mutation-grossbritannien-ansteckung.